Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
Neuropsychiatry (London). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as:
Neuropsychiatry (London). 2013 Oct 1; 3(5): 455–458.
doi:  10.2217/npy.13.59
PMCID: PMC3955126
NIHMSID: NIHMS552575

Changes in the Definition of ADHD in DSM-5: Subtle but Important

While there were earlier descriptions of children with high levels of activity and impulsivity [1], what is now called Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) first appeared in the second edition of the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders in 1968 [2]. In DSM-II, the disorder was termed Hyperkinetic Reaction of Childhood, which as the name implies focused primarily on symptoms of excessive motor activity. With the publication of the DSM-III [3] in 1980, the disorder was markedly re-conceptualized with a focus on problems with attention, impulsivity and hyperactivity, and was renamed Attention Deficit Disorder (with and without Hyperactivity). The term Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) was introduced in DSM-III-R [4], with the controversial elimination of ADD without Hyperactivity. With the publication of the DSM- IV [5], the term ADHD was retained along with the introduction of three specific subtypes (predominantly Inattentive, predominantly Hyperactive-Impulsive, and Combined), defined by the presence of excessive symptoms of inattention and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity.

The recent release of DSM-5 [6] is the latest update to ADHD nosology. The DSM-5 revisions include modifications to each of the ADHD diagnostic criteria (A-E), a terminological change in the ADHD subtype nosology, and the addition of two ADHD modifiers. Criterion A (ADHD symptoms) are unchanged from DSM-IV except for additional examples of how symptoms may manifest in adolescence and adulthood, and a reduction from six to five in the minimum number of symptoms in either symptom domain required for older adolescents and adults. Criterion B (age of onset) changed from onset of symptoms and impairments before age 7 to onset of symptoms before age 12. Criterion C (pervasiveness) was changed from evidence of impairment to evidence of symptoms in two or more settings. Criterion D (impairment) now requires that functional impairments only need to “reduce the quality of social, academic or occupational functioning” instead of requiring that they be “clinically significant.” Criterion E (exclusionary conditions) no longer includes Autism Spectrum Disorder as an exclusionary diagnosis. Regarding nosology, the DSM-IV ADHD “types” are now referred to as “presentations.” Finally, modifiers were added so that the severity of the disorder (i.e., mild, moderate, or severe) can be specified and the disorder can be coded as “in partial remission” if full diagnostic criteria are not currently met.

Overall, the revisions to ADHD in DSM-5 are less dramatic than updates to earlier DSMs. Importantly, the DSM-5 ADHD and Disruptive Behavior Disorders Workgroup decided neither to modify the core ADHD symptom domains (i.e., Inattention and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity) nor to revise the 18 core symptoms, aside from adding example behaviors to better define some of the symptoms for older adolescents and adults. The retention of the ADHD symptom domains and 18 core symptoms likely reflects a judgment that the DSM-IV definition of ADHD has largely withstood the test of time. DSM-IV ADHD criteria have proven to be quite effective at reliably identifying a population of individuals who have significant impairments across a wide range of outcomes (e.g., academic, interpersonal, occupational, personal, substance use, driving, etc. [7, 8]). Moreover, individuals identified by DSM-IV ADHD criteria appear to have distinct neuropsychological profiles [9, 10] identifiable neurobiological signatures (e.g., abnormalities in frontal-striatal circuitry [11, 12]) and unique genetic correlates [13]. By retaining a similar ADHD phenotype as defined in DSM-IV, the DSM-5 workgroup ensured that the voluminous body of DSM-IV defined ADHD research accumulated over the past 2 decades will largely generalize to the new, yet highly similar, DSM-5 ADHD phenotype.

Although more subtle than changes in prior DSMs, the changes to ADHD in DSM-5 are important and reflect our increased knowledge about the nature of ADHD. In particular, it has become increasingly evident that the DSM-IV symptom domain thresholds (i.e., 6 of 9 symptoms per symptom domain), while appropriate for young children, are not effective for identifying adolescents and adults experiencing ADHD-related impairment. Prior to DSM-5, some researchers used lower symptom thresholds to define adolescent and/or adult ADHD samples (e.g., [14]), in discord with DSM-IV; many clinicians did likewise or relied on the poorly-defined ADHD Not Otherwise Specified. Research suggests that a lower symptom number threshold more accurately identifies those 17 and older who experience impairments warranting intervention [15].

Similarly, research has shown no meaningful differences in functioning, response to treatment, or outcomes in individuals who display ADHD symptoms prior to age 7 versus those who first display symptoms at an older age [16]. Both research and clinical experience indicates some ADHD patient groups (e.g., those with high intelligence, with predominantly inattentive symptoms, or in a highly structured environment) may not experience significant impairment until expectations for self- management increase in late elementary or middle school. For those individuals whose ADHD is not identified until adulthood, they often have difficulty recalling at what age they first experienced impairments, as the inherent memory problems often associated with ADHD make recall of childhood details difficult. The change to an age of onset of 12, while albeit still rather arbitrary, may reduce some of these diagnostic issues.

The change in nomenclature from “subtypes” in DSM-IV to “presentations” in DSM-5 reflects increasing evidence that symptoms are often fluid within individuals across their lifespan rather than stable traits. DSM-IV ADHD subtypes change across development due to the heterotypic continuity of symptom trajectories over time. For example, since inattention is relatively stable across development while hyperactivity/impulsivity often wane with age, many children diagnosed with ADHD, Combined eventually transition to ADHD, Predominantly Inattentive [17]. The “presentation” terminology better reflects that the symptom profile represents the person’s current symptomatology, which may change over time. The “type” terminology implied more stable, trait-like characteristics. Finally, modifying Criteria E to allow a diagnosis of ADHD comorbid with ASD is consistent with research indicating that children with ASD can also have ADHD [18].

Besides aligning the ADHD criteria with the current state of knowledge, the modifications in DSM-5 have the potential to make the ADHD diagnosis more reliable. In particular, the switch from requiring evidence of impairing symptoms to just symptoms for both the pervasiveness and age of onset criteria likely improves their reliability. Symptoms tend to be more easily quantified and observed. There are numerous established measures of ADHD symptoms, whereas impairments tend to be more qualitative and subjective for which we have fewer reliable measures. However, since ADHD symptoms can exist in the absence of impairment, whereas impairments in the absence of symptoms are unlikely, focusing on symptoms without impairments may increase the number of children who meet both age of onset and pervasiveness criteria. In addition, the modification of the definition of impairment from “significant” to “interfere with, reduce the quality of…” is also a more liberal and more inclusive requirement. So, while the new DSM-5 ADHD criteria may result in a more reliable set of criteria, ADHD prevalence rates may increase.

A couple of issues were unfortunately not addressed in the revisions to ADHD in DSM-5. First, there is an increasing, if not universal, acceptance that ADHD, like many psychopathologies, is a dimensional disorder [19]. That is, inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity are behavioral traits that naturally occur on a continuum, much like intelligence. In this view, diagnostic thresholds used to define “abnormal behavior” are artificial, though useful in identifying individuals who experience significant impairment in their daily functioning. DSM-5 continues to place everyone meeting diagnostic criteria into a single category which doesn’t capture the dimensionality of underlying constructs. While DSM-5 does allow for a severity classification (mild, moderate, or severe), these can be applied based on either number of symptoms or magnitude of impairment. Given that both symptom counts and functional impairment can, and often do, vary across domains and across settings, it is likely that severity classifications will be unreliable and will vary considerably across diagnosticians. Preferably, some form of indication of level of global functioning might most accurately indicate severity of the disorder. The WHO Disability Assessment Scale (WHODAS) has been added to DSM-5, and is somewhat akin to indicating global functioning except it assesses the impact of the patient’s entire diagnostic profile on global functioning. Future revisions should consider other nosological devices to indicate both the dimensionality of the disorder and the impact of each specific disorder (e.g., ADHD) on overall functioning. Perhaps overall clinical global impressions [20], as used to assess severity of impairment in ADHD clinical trials, could be considered.

Finally, while some changes, as noted above, were made to make the ADHD criteria more applicable to older adolescents and adults, the DSM-5 ADHD diagnostic structure fails to reflect established developmental trajectories. In particular, the “Predominantly Inattentive Presentation” includes both children who, at a younger age, met criteria for “Combined presentation” as well as those who have always had few, if any, Hyperactive/Impulsive symptoms. Pre-release correspondence from the Workgroup suggested that an “Inattentive Restrictive Type” was considered for children with consistently low numbers of hyperactive/impulsive symptoms. It is possible that segmenting out this sub-population of children with ADHD may have both addressed heterogeneity within the ADHD Predominantly Inattentive presentation and spurred research into whether specific symptom trajectories are associated with prognosis, neurobiological correlates, comorbidity patterns, etc. Hopefully, future revisions will reconsider such sub-classifications, or other strategies for capturing developmental changes over time.

Footnotes

Financial Disclosure

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References

1. Still G. Some abnormal psychical conditions in children. Lancet. 1902;1:1008–1012. 1077-1082. 1163-1168.
2. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 2nd edition (DSM-II) Author; Washington, D.C.: 1968.
3. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd edition (DSM-III) Author; Washington, D.C.: 1980.
4. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd edition. Author; Washington, DC.: 1987. revised (DSM-3-R)
5. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th edition. Author; Washington, DC.: 1994.
6. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th edition. Author; Washington, DC.: 2013.
7. Barkley RA. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment. The Guilford Press; New York: 2006.
8. Willcutt EG, Nigg JT, Pennington BF, et al. Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2012;121:991–1010. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
9. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF. Validity of the executive function theory of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biol. Psychiatry. 2005;57:1336–1346. [PubMed]
10. Hervey AS, Epstein JN, Curry JF. The neuropsychology of adults with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. 2004;18:485–503. [PubMed]
11. Valera EM, Faraone SV, Murray KE, Seidman LJ. Meta-analysis of structural imaging findings in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;61:1361–1369. [PubMed]
12. Swanson JM, Kinsbourne M, Nigg J, et al. Etiologic subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: brain imaging, molecular genetic and environmental factors and the dopamine hypothesis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2007;17:39–59. [PubMed]
13. Gizer IR, Ficks C, Waldman ID. Candidate gene studies of ADHD: a meta-analytic review. Hum. Genet. 2009;126(1):51–90. [PubMed]
14. Barkley RA. Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2012;121:978–990. [PubMed]
15. Kooij JJ, Buitelaar JK, Van Den Oord EJ, Furer JW, Rijnders CA, Hodiamont PP. Internal and external validity of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in a population-based sample of adults. Psychol. Med. 2005;35:817–827. [PubMed]
16. Barkley R, Biederman J. Toward a broader definition of the age-of-onset criterion for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry . 1997;36:1204–1210. [PubMed]
17. Hurtig T, Ebeling H, Taanila A, Al. E. ADHD symptoms and subtypes: Relationship between childhood and adolescent symptoms. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2007;46:1605–1613. [PubMed]
18. Simonoff E, Pickles A, Charman T, Chandler S, Loucas T, Baird G. Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2008;47:921–929. [PubMed]
19. Lubke GH, Hudziak JJ, Derks EM, Van Bijsterveldt TC, Boomsma DI. Maternal ratings of attention problems in ADHD: evidence for the existence of a continuum. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2009;48:1085–1093. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
20. Guy W. ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology. US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare; Bethesda: 1976. Revised.